Fluid Intelligence Test Scores Across the Schooling: Evidence of Nonlinear Changes in Girls and Boys
Abstract
The results of the analyses of the changes of fluid intelligence scores measured by the Standard Progressive Matrices test across all school years were presented. Sex differences in fluid intelligence scores for each year of schooling as well as in fluid intelligence changes across schooling were analyzed. A total of 1581 participants (51.1% boys) aged 6.8 to 19.1 years from one public school were involved in this cross-sectional study, of whom 871 were primary schoolchildren (mean age = 9.23; range 6.8–11.6), 507 were secondary schoolchildren (mean age = 14.06; range 10.8–18.0), and 203 were high schoolchildren (mean age = 17.25; range 15.3–19.1). To examine the changes in fluid intelligence both correlation analysis and polynomial regression of the total, boys’ and girls’ samples were performed. Linear, quadratic, and cubic regression models were fitted to the data. To explore sex differences in fluid intelligence in each year of schooling, the series of ANOVA were carried out. The results revealed that the school-age change in fluid intelligence is nonlinear for both girls and boys. The changes for girls during the schooling are best described by a quadratic relationship while those for boys are best reflected by a cubic relationship.
References
- Aichele, S., Rabbitt, P., & Ghisletta, P. (2019). Illness and intelligence are comparatively strong predictors of individual differences in depressive symptoms following middle age. Aging & Mental Health, 23(1), 122–131. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2017.1394440
- Baltes, P., & Reinert, G. (1969). Cohort effects in cognitive development of children as revealed by cross-sectional sequences. Developmental Psychology, 1(2), 169–177. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026997
- Barbey, A. K. (2018). Network neuroscience theory of human intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 8–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.10.001
- Brinch, C. N., & Galloway, T. A. (2012). Schooling in adolescence raises IQ scores. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(2), 425–430. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1106077109
- Brouwers, S. A., van de Vijver, F. J. R., & van Hemert, D. A. (2009). Variation in Raven’s Progressive Matrices scores across time and place. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(3), 330–338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2008.10.006
- Brown, R. E. (2016). Hebb and Cattell: The genesis of the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, Article 606. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00606
- Cahan, S., & Cohen, N. (1989). Age versus schooling effects on intelligence development. Child Development, 60(5), 1239–1249. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130797
- Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046743
- Colom, R., & Lynn, R. (2004). Testing the developmental theory of sex differences in intelligence on 12–18 year olds. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(1), 75–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00053-9
- Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith P., & Fernandes, C. (2007). Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence, 35(1), 13–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001
- Deary, I. J., & Johnson, W. (2010). Intelligence and education: Causal perceptions drive analytic processes and therefore conclusions. International Journal of Epidemiology, 39(5), 1362–1369. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyq072
- Desjardins, R., & Warnke, A. J. (2012). Ageing and skills: A review and analysis of skill gain and skill loss over the lifespan and over time (OECD Education Working Papers, No. 72). OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/5k9csvw87ckh-en
- Flynn, J. R., & Rossi-Casé, L. (2011). Modern women match men on Raven’s Progressive Matrices. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(6), 799–803. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.12.035
- Frenken, H., Papageorgiou, K. A., Tikhomirova, T., Malykh, S., Tosto, M. D., & Kovas, Y. (2016). Siblings’ sex is linked to mental rotation performance in males but not females. Intelligence, 55, 38–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.01.005
- Ghisletta, P., Rabbitt, P., Lunn, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2012). Two thirds of the age-based changes in fluid and crystallized intelligence, perceptual speed, and memory in adulthood are shared. Intelligence, 40(3), 260–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.02.008
- Hartshorne, J., & Germine, L. (2015). When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychological Science, 26(4), 433–443. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614567339
- Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1966) Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57(5), 253–270. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0023816
- Irwing, P., & Lynn, R. (2005). Sex differences in means and variability on the progressive matrices in university students: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 96(4), 505–524. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712605X53542
- Jackson, M., Khavenson, T., & Chirkina, T. (2020). Raising the stakes: Inequality and testing in the Russian education system. Social Forces, 98(4), 1613–1635. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/soz113
- Kievit, R. A., Davis, S. W., Griffiths, J., Correia, M. M., Cam-CAN, & Henson, R. N. (2016). A watershed model of individual differences in fluid intelligence. Neuropsychologia, 91, 186–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.008
- Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2014). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 37–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.011
- Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67(2), 130–159. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026699
- Ob obrazovanii v Rossiiskoi Federatsii [On Education in the Russian Federation]. Federal Law of the Russian Federation No. 273-FZ (2012, December 29, rev. on 2021, July 2). http://pravo.gov.ru/proxy/ips/?docbody=&nd=102162745
- Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science, 306(5695), 499–503. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1102085
- Raven, J. J. (2003). Raven Progressive Matrices. In R. S. McCallum (Ed.), Handbook of nonverbal assessment (pp. 223–237). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0153-4_11
- Ritchie, S. J., Bates, T. C., & Deary, I. J. (2015). Is education associated with improvements in general cognitive ability, or in specific skills? Developmental Psychology, 51(5), 573–582. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038981
- Schneeweis, N., Skirbekk, V., & Winter-Ebmer, R. (2014). Does education improve cognitive performance four decades after school completion? Demography, 51(2), 619–643. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-014-0281-1
- Shangguan, F., & Shi, J. (2009). Puberty timing and fluid intelligence: A study of correlations between testosterone and intelligence in 8- to 12-year-old Chinese boys. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(7), 983–988. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.01.012
- Tikhomirova, T., Kuzmina, Y., Lysenkova, I., & Malykh, S. (2019a). Development of approximate number sense across the elementary school years: A cross-cultural longitudinal study. Developmental Science, 22(4), e12823. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12823
- Tikhomirova, T., Kuzmina, Y., Lysenkova, I., & Malykh, S. (2019b). The relationship between non-symbolic and symbolic numerosity representations in elementary school: The role of intelligence. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(2019), Article 2724. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02724
- Tikhomirova, T., Malykh, A., & Malykh, S. (2020). Predicting academic achievement with cognitive abilities: Cross-sectional study across school education. Behavioral Sciences, 10(10), Article 158. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs10100158
- Tucker-Drob, E., & Briley, D. (2014). Continuity of genetic and environmental influences on cognition across the life span: A meta-analysis of longitudinal twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 140(4), 949–979. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035893
- von Stumm, S., & Plomin, R. (2015). Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence. Intelligence, 48, 30–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.10.002
- Zilles, D., Lewandowski, M., Vieker, H., Henseler, I., Diekhof, E., Melcher, T., Keil, M., & Gruber, O. (2016). Gender differences in verbal and visuospatial working memory performance and networks. Neuropsychobiology, 73(1), 52–63. https://doi.org/10.1159/000443174